CLASSIFYING STUDENT |
IN THE INTRODUCTION TO |
MICROECONOMICS COURSE |
By Charles Ellard, Martin Feinberg, and Jeffrey Sam Siekpe
Charles Ellard cjeb12b@panam.edu
Abstract A system to classify student errors is especially useful in introductory economic classes. This paper develops a system of classifying student errors from essay exam answers in the context of an introductory microeconomics course. Some tendency for students to repeat errors of diagramming dealing with missing parts and to make logical errors in graphing after a successful free graph on an earlier test was found. |
INTRODUCTION
Classifying
student errors is beneficial in a pedagogical sense. It is perhaps a unique
approach to economic education to focus on student errors, rather than measures
of achievement. However, they are, in fact, complements to each other. One of
the advantages of studying learning dysfunctions is that we can more readily
identify problem areas and hence solutions to problems. Every instructor who
sifts through student essays on economic applications immediately identifies
mistakes and problems for that moment, and he or she is usually able to make judgments
and evaluations on the seriousness of the error.
Rarely
are the essay exams themselves looked at as a valid observation of student
understanding to be scrutinized and recorded. On the other hand, instructors
using multiple choice exams often perform item analysis on the various choices
students make in order to determine which of the incorrect answers distracted
the student enough to make the wrong choice. Item analysis of this type can an
important investigation of student errors in order to improve exam questions.
However, it may lead to simply making more clever and tricky distractors;
which is not exactly conducive to learning. The point here is that the
analysis of student errors on tests can provide the means to improve tests of
that nature.
In
this
paper we seek to develop a system of classifying student errors from essay exam
answers in the context of an introductory economics course.
Since the response is not finite as in the multiple choice test, a
system of classification of errors must be developed. However, in this case the
use of the information is not so much to make a better test, but to try to
understand the nature of student thinking so that instruction can be improved.
This
paper describes the subject of economic ideas upon which the error
classification system is aimed. It is identified and its importance
established. Subsequently, a classification system is developed and an
application is described. The results of the application are then analyzed and
some conclusions are drawn.
A
variety of researches encompassing eclectic academic disciplines have
attempted to devise classification grading schemes. Lee (1990) proposes a
system for classifying errors in linguistics and language teaching theory. The
author suggested four complementary ways of categorizing errors: error vs.
mistake; linguistic phenomenon; gravity of the error; and difficulty of
correction. Hendrickson (1979) depicts a method for classifying errors of
second language learners. Druger (2000) introduced
grading classification in the natural sciences in his study dealing
with introductory college biology.
The theory of
classifying errors is cogently analyzed in Valentine (1982). The author
emphasizes the importance of sufficient evidence of discriminant validity in
regards to evaluating errors classification schemes. Durfee (1993) emphasizes
the need for positive feedback in regards to developing errors classification
schemes. Livingston and Lewis (1995) introduced the concept of classification
consistency and classification accuracy, whereas Hoffman and Wise (2000)
introduced the concept of classification reliability.
The
competitive market model is a fundamental part of most introductory economics
courses and the texts which are used. This
material generally includes a coverage of demand and supply, laying out
the ceteris paribus demand curve including the principle of the law of demand.
Market price and quantity determination is covered in schedules and graphs and
the equilibrium process completes a verbal description of price determination.
The uses of the model include a demonstration of reasons for price movements
utilizing the relaxation of the ceteris paribus assumption for the various
non- price factors of demand and supply. The list of these factors is fairly
standard; income, price of related goods, number of buyers, tastes and
preferences and expectations on the demand side; cost of resources, number of
sellers, technology and expectations on the supply side. This model of the product market is also utilized to illustrate effects
of price fixing arrangement and
tax burden. Elasticity measures
allow certain generalizations about market operations, especially when
connected to revenue generated by the market.
Most
introductory courses continue the development of the market system by
illustrating the circular flow mechanism and the operation of factor markets
including especially labor, and less frequently markets for capital and
natural resources. These markets form the basis for a consideration of the
functional distribution of income. Details of the role of profit round out the
distribution aspects of the circular flow. The treatment of these aspects of
the market are generally static with partial equilibrium structures utilized
to demonstrate basic principles.
The
understanding of the competitive market model described above is
normally high on the priority list of learning objectives for the introductory
economics course. While the
introductory economics course typically shares the semester calendar with
various macro topics, it is clear that with enough choices of topics to fill
several semester calendars the competitive market model is the topic of choice
for most economics instructors. It
is clearly important in understanding the economic environment in
which we live.
Instructors
may differ in the method and approach to teaching the competitive market
model. Some may treat it as strictly an abstract cognitive exercise;
others may insist on the development of applications under different
circumstances. The premise of this paper is that the competitive market model
is important enough with wide and diverse applicability I that students
should be required to demonstrate competence in using it as a tool of
analysis. This implies competence in correctly utilizing supply and demand
diagrams and adequately explaining its conclusions and implications.
The
focus of this paper is on the classification and analysis of learning
dysfunctions of students of the competitive market model. In particular the
learning dysfunctions involve a large number of observable errors and
characteristics of student answers to short word problems involving the supply
and demand model. The list of errors includes 20 observable characteristics of
the diagram submitted by the student in response to the word problem
and seven characteristics of the written portion of the answer.
These
27 elements comprise the scope of the error set analyzed in this paper. From
the classification of these 27 error types, it is necessary to proceed to
meaningful analysis. Some aggregation will aid the analysis. The error types
are collected into groups of similar errors in order to concentrate the data
and make statistical analysis more meaningful. The error types are compiled
into three major groups and a total or overall error
category. These are:
Failure to attempt an
answer
Diagram
errors
Serious problems of written expression
The failure to attempt an answer involves leaving
out all or a portion of what the question asks for.
The obvious cases are where there is a diagram present with no written
essay, or a written answer without a diagram.
Turning in a blank page is also an indication of this type of error. The failure to attempt an answer includes observations regarding the
students’ answer indicating frustration and impatience and uncertainty in
formulating an answer. This
category includes extreme sloppiness of a diagram, one or more false starts or
erasures, extreme grammatical faults that impede evaluation, and extremely
sloppy penmanship that impedes evaluation.
Diagram
errors include a wide variety of faults regarding the construction of the
diagram. Diagrams in these
particular word problems involve a one-factor relaxation of the ceteris
paribus assumption on a normal supply and demand presentation either in the
product market or the factor market. Errors include missing arts, logical
errors, labeling detail faults, and others. The missing parts error pertains to student presented diagrams which
were odd or abnormal, where no shift is indicated but the supply and demand
remains intact, or where there is a missing supply or a missing demand curve.
Logical
errors include instances where: the wrong curve is shifted; the correct
curve is shifted in the wrong direction; both supply and demand are
shifted in a one shift problem; equilibrium price and quantity are out
of correspondence with supply-demand intersections; supply and demand
are placed on different graphs; and the students focus is on the wrong
market in a problem involving substitutes or complements. Labeling problems also plague student response, though they may not be
as serious as some other types of errors.
Labeling mistakes include supply and demand curves labeling reversed;
odd-ball labels on supply or demand or on the axes which are left unexplained
by the student; and made up numbers placed on the axes which are unrelated to the
problem and other similar errors.
Written expression errors involve characteristics
of the written portion of the response with the following possible
characteristics: extreme difficulty in stating cause and effect in the context
of a one-factor shift in the market analysis; minimized verbal expression,
that is, being too brief or crimped in the response so that it does not
sufficiently underscore the diagrammatic analysis; and excessive verbiage
which is incorrect, otherwise referred to as "shotgunning" or
guessing.
Obviously
any given student may not have many or even any of these error characteristics.
It is only by analyzing the totality of these error patterns that we might
learn something and be able to make teaching recommendations on how to assist
students in minimizing errors.
This
classification scheme was applied to an introductory economics class at
the University of Texas-Pan American. The analysis which follows is
based on these observations. The course was roughly divided into two
parts, the
first of which dealt with a variety of topics but focused on the
microeconomics of market analysis, including both product and factor markets. The second half dealt with macro topics, including output, prices,
unemployment and the like and focused on the aggregate supply and demand
analyses.
The
analysis presented here deals only with the micro portions of the analysis.
The observations made in the application of the error classification
system described above involved part of the examinations that were given.
There were several questions involved, one in each of two exams.
These included a question on Exam 1 that involved the
simple product market model of supply and demand and a two-part question on
Exam 2 that involved the application of the market model for the
product and one related to a related factor market--labor in
this case. This two-part problem was meant to involve the
interrelatedness of product and factor markets.
Students were schooled only on immediate impacts and were not expected
to trace out general equilibrium repercussions on related product or factor
markets. Additionally, at the end of the semester students were given an
opportunity to retake either exam with a second chance test which
covered similar material as was on the first test. Students were given the option to
take this make up test or not; except those who had missed the earlier test
were required to take the make up.
RESULTS
Observations were taken on all word problem answers
provided by the students in each of these exams. The specific wording of the
word problems included is included in Appendix A. Classification summaries
were made according to the system described above and dummy variables were
employed. Table 1 presents the total errors by classification for the
introductory economics class under observation.
The
analysis which follows attempts to answer the question; when do students make
errors? The question of "when" errors are made focuses on the
issue of whether students tend to repeat the same errors in subsequent tests.
Here an association is made between Test 1 and Test 2 in terms
of the type of error.
The
fact that just about all students make errors at some time is not
surprising; it becomes a task for student and instructor to learn from
those mistakes and make the necessary adjustments, since these errors include
less and more serious types of errors. It would be expected that even the
better students make some errors.
It
is also of interest as to whether student errors are persistent: do students
repeat error?. Table 2 is a tabulation of all errors by
type for the two exams relating to the micro concepts of the market
model of supply and demand and the two makeup tests. These data indicate that
in terms of total number of errors made, there is a drop of about 20 percent from the
first to the second exam. However,
this drop is entirely due to errors of expression not being repeated. Other
types of errors: failure to attempt and diagram errors, stayed about the same
from the first to the second test.
However,
these
totals do not tell the whole story. What is really of interest is
whether or not any given student is repeating the same type of mistake. Taking Exams 1 and 2 together, four possibilities might be present.
(Where 0 = no error, 1 = error)
Exam 1 |
Exam 2 |
|
A |
0 |
0 |
B |
0 |
1 |
C |
1 |
0 |
D |
1 |
1 |
Table 3 presents the cell counts
for these four possibilities; with respect to the different error types. Under
column A are those who made no error in either Exam l or Exam 2. The B column
includes those who made no errors on the first exam, but made an error on the
second. Since both tests involve
applications of the basic market model, this category represents to some
extent students who possessed the skills on the first exam but did not retain
them. This is referred to as the forget factor. Under columns C and D
are the number of students who had an error on the first exam. Column C
indicates out of those how many did not repeat the error, while column
D indicates those that repeated the error.
Two ratios are included in Table 3; the forget factor or the percent of
those who made no errors on Exam 1 in any given error category who made
an error on Exam 2. The other ratio is the repeat factor or the
percent of those making an error on Exam 1 who made the same type error
on Exam 2. Overall, students had a high repeat factor 58.3
percent, that
is, if they had an error of any type on Exam 1 then almost six out of ten also has
an error of some type on Exam 2.
The tendency to make errors on
the second exam when the first was error free, or the forget factor, was most
prevalent in diagram errors. In
particular, the logical errors related to diagramming. Mistakes such as shifting the wrong curve or the right curve in the
wrong direction may appear in the second test with new situational
description. It may also be due to
forgetting the details of the model. Also,
the sloppiness errors had a 24.1 percent forget error factor which was also quite
high. This is surprising since
neatness or sloppiness is usually thought to be habitual in nature. Perhaps
the high forget factor in this category is more related to the frustration
coming through, of facing a more trying test the second time around. Otherwise, the forget factors quite low for other types of errors.
This
paper has attempted to develop a classification system for categorizing
student errors on word problems associated with the supply and demand market
model. This classification system
was applied in a class of introductory economics students at UT-Pan American. The indications from this application are that most students make
errors at one time or another in the class.
There was some tendency for students to repeat errors of diagramming
dealing with missing parts and to make logical errors in graphing after a
successful error free graph on an earlier test.
Future
research should increase the sample size in order to improve the external
validity of the classification system. It might also be important to
consider some screening method at the beginning of the course to identify
those students who might be predicted to have difficulty with graphic
analysis, and instead of moving on, have a remedial or study session
with that group to review some of the basics of graphs.
For
the general student group it might be helpful if the course objectives
point
SUMMARY OF STUDENTS ERRORS ACCORDING TO THE ERROR CLASSIFICATION SYSTEM
(n=36)
LEVEL |
DEFINITION |
NUMBER
OF STUDENTS MADE ERROR AT LEAST ONCE |
NUMBER
OF STUDENTS NEVER MADE ERROR |
1 |
Frustration |
15 |
21 |
1A |
Failure
to attempt |
1 |
35 |
1B |
Sloppiness
|
14 |
22 |
2 |
Diagram
errors |
29 |
7 |
2A |
Missing
parts |
9 |
27 |
2B |
Logical
errors |
21 |
15 |
2C |
Labeling
detail faults |
10 |
26 |
2D |
Other |
6 |
30 |
3 |
Written
expression problems |
12 |
24 |
3A |
Major
problems in expression |
12 |
24 |
|
Any
error |
31 |
5 |
TOTAL ERROR RATES BY
EXAM FOR THE DIFFERENT ERROR CATEGORIES
|
|
EXAM
1 |
EXAM
2 |
MAKEUP
1 |
MAKEUP
2 |
LEVEL |
ERROR
CATEGORY |
(36) |
(35) |
(17) |
(8) |
1 |
Frustration |
9 |
9 |
1 |
0 |
1A |
Failure
to attempt |
1 |
1 |
0 |
0 |
1B |
Sloppiness
|
8 |
8 |
1 |
0 |
2 |
Diagram
errors |
21 |
22 |
4 |
8 |
2A |
Missing
parts |
6 |
5 |
0 |
1 |
2B |
Logical
errors |
9 |
12 |
3 |
2 |
2C |
Labeling
detail faults |
5 |
3 |
1 |
2 |
2D |
Other |
1 |
2 |
0 |
3 |
3 |
Written
expression problems |
11 |
2 |
2 |
0 |
3A |
Major
problems in expression |
11 |
2 |
2 |
0 |
|
Any
error |
41 |
33 |
7 |
8 |
DESCRIPTION OF ERROR PATTERNS FOR EXAM 1 AND EXAM 2
|
|
|
|
|
|
FORGET
FACTOR |
REPEAT
FACTOR |
LEVEL |
ERROR
CATEGORY |
A |
B |
C |
D |
||
1 |
Frustration |
21 |
8 |
6 |
1 |
27.6% |
14.2% |
1A |
Failure
to attempt |
35 |
1 |
0 |
0 |
2.8 |
- |
1B |
Sloppiness
|
22 |
7 |
6 |
1 |
24.1 |
14.2 |
2 |
Diagram
errors |
9 |
10 |
10 |
7 |
52.6 |
41.2 |
2A |
Missing
parts |
28 |
2 |
3 |
3 |
6.7 |
50.0 |
2B |
Logical
errors |
17 |
11 |
7 |
1 |
39.3 |
12.5 |
2C |
Labeling
detail faults |
28 |
3 |
5 |
0 |
9.7 |
0.0 |
2D |
Other |
32 |
2 |
1 |
0 |
5.9 |
0.0 |
3 |
Written
expression problems |
24 |
1 |
10 |
1 |
4.0 |
9.1 |
3A |
Major
problems in expression |
24 |
1 |
10 |
1 |
4.0 |
9.1 |
|
Any
error |
6 |
6 |
10 |
14 |
50.0 |
58.3 |
Forget
Factor = B/(A +B)
Repeat Factor = D/(C+D)
EXAM
QUESTIONS
MICRO
EXAM 1:
Suppose the price of coffee decreased, analyze the impact in the market
for tea. Explain in words and using a supply and demand diagram.
EXAM 2: Suppose medical science determines that a person taking grapefruit
pectin, which is a product made from the rind of a grapefruit, lowers blood
cholesterol dramatically; and consequently, will reduce the probability of heart
attacks. As this idea catches on, what will the impact be on the market for
grapefruit? Explain and illustrate.
Follow
the economic effects of the problem discussed above on the market for citrus
workers in the key citrus growing regions, such as
EXAM
1 - Second Chance:
EXAM
2 - Second Chance:
Given the above what would happen to the market for longshoremen and
dock workers in
Druger,
Marvin (2000). “A Perspective on Exams and Grading.” Journal of College
Science and Teaching, Vol. 30, Issue 3, 210-211.
Hendrickson, James M. (1979).
“Evaluating Spontaneous Communication Through Systematic Error Analysis.” Foreign
Language Annals, Vol. 12, No. 5, 357-364.
Hoffman, R. Gene, and Wise, Lauress L.
(April 2000). “Establishing the Reliability of Student Proficiency
Classifications: The Accuracy of Observed Classifications.” Paper presented at
the annual meeting of the National Council of Measurement in Education,
Lee,
Livingston, S. A., and Lewis, C.
(1995). “Estimating the Consistency and Accuracy of Classifications Based on
Test Scores.” Journal of Education Measurement, Vol. 32, No. 2,
179-197.
Valentine, Thomas (1982). “An
Examination of the Validity of Two Item Classification Schemes for the
B>Quest
(Business Quest)
A journal of applied topics in business and economics